Mood and cognition after administration of low LSD doses in healthy volunteers: A placebo controlled dose-effect finding study

This fourth publication on the administration of a microdose of LSD (5, 10, or 20 µg) found enhanced attention, slower information processing, more positive mood, and increased anxiety and confusion. Again, the results are small and quite ambiguous.

Abstract

“There is a popular interest in microdosing with psychedelics such as LSD. This practice of using one-tenth of a full psychedelic dose according to a specific dosing schedule, anecdotally enhances mood and performance. Nonetheless, controlled research on the efficacy of microdosing is scarce. The main objective of the present dose-finding study was to determine the minimal dose of LSD needed to affect mood and cognition. A placebo-controlled within-subject study including 24 healthy participants, was conducted to assess the acute effects of three LSD doses (5, 10, and 20 mcg) on measures of cognition, mood, and subjective experience, up until 6 h after administration. Cognition and subjective experience were assessed using the Psychomotor Vigilance Task, Digit Symbol Substitution Test, Cognitive Control Task, Profile of Mood States, and 5-Dimensional Altered States of Consciousness rating scale. LSD showed positive effects in the majority of observations by increasing positive mood (20 mcg), friendliness (5, 20 mcg), arousal (5 mcg), and decreasing attentional lapses (5, 20 mcg). Negative effects manifested as an increase in confusion (20 mcg) and anxiety (5, 20 mcg). Psychedelic-induced changes in waking consciousness were also present (10, 20 mcg). Overall, the present study demonstrated selective, beneficial effects of low doses of LSD on mood and cognition in the majority of observations. The minimal LSD dose at which subjective and performance effects are notable is 5 mcg and the most apparent effects were visible after 20 mcg.”

Authors: Nadia R. P. W. Hutten, Natasha L. Mason, Patrick C. Dolder, Eef L. Theunissen, Friederike Holze, Matthias E. Liechti, Amanda Feilding, Johannes G. Ramaekers & Kim P. C. Kuypers

Notes

This paper uses the same participants (possibly now burdened with RSI) as Holze et al. (2020), Hutten et al. (2020), and Ramaekers et al. (2020). The other studies respectively looked at pharmacodynamics and felt effect (only really at 20 µg), blood plasma levels (ambiguous results), and increased pain tolerance.

This study is quite similar to Yanakieva et al. (2019), which found only small effects on mood (more vigour, lower rating of positive images).

The study is supported in part by the Beckley Foundation.

It was (lazily) reported on in Futurism (October 2020).

Although the conclusions presented in the abstract are valid. Please do consult Figure 1 in the paper to see how small and relatively ‘random’ these results are (i.e. noise). Also:

Binomial tests indicated that the proportion of observations containing fewer attentional lapses after administration of 5 and 20 mcg LSD was respectively 76% ( p < 0.01) and 74% ( p < 0.01), while this was 38% ( p = 0.19) after 10 mcg LS.” (this is represented in the top-left bar graph in figure 1)

The same can be said of Table 3, which displays mood and cognition scores that move only ever so slightly.

Summary

A placebo-controlled within-subject study was conducted to assess the acute effects of three LSD doses on measures of cognition, mood, and subjective experience. The results demonstrated that low doses of LSD have selective, beneficial effects on mood and cognition.

2.1. Design

This study was conducted with 24 participants and involved four doses of LSD hydrate. The treatment days were separated by a minimum washout period of 5 days to avoid potential carry-over effects.

2.2. Participants

Participants were 24 healthy recreational psychedelic drug users aged 22.8 years on average. They had used at least one psychedelic substance, and reported previous use of cannabis, amphetamines, cocaine, nicotine, and alcohol.

2.3. Procedures

Participants were recruited through advertisements, social media and word of mouth. They underwent a medical screening by a physician and blood and urine samples were taken.

Inclusion criteria were written informed consent, proficient knowledge of the English language, and a BMI between 18 and 28 kg/m 2.

Exclusion criteria were history of drug abuse, psychiatric disorders, cardiovascular abnormalities, hypertension, psychotic disorder in first-degree relatives, tobacco smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, and pregnancy.

Participants were trained to perform with 100% accuracy on a rule-based cognitive control task (CCT) before the test days. They were also asked to refrain from consuming caffeinated or alcoholic beverages after midnight of the evening before the test days.

Participants spent the whole day in a quiet room with a window, were screened for the presence of alcohol in breath, drugs of abuse in urine, and women were tested for pregnancy. After drug administration, a test battery was conducted, including subjective measures and blood samples.

This study was conducted according to the code of ethics on human experimentation established by the declaration of Helsinki and subsequent amendments, and participants received monetary compensation per hour invested.

2.4.1. Psychomotor vigilance task

The Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) is a sustained attention task in which participants have to react as quickly as possible to visual stimuli.

2.4.2. Digit symbol substitution test

The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) is a computerized version of the original paper and pencil test that measures information processing, motor speed, attention, working memory, and visual processing.

2.4.3. Cognitive control task

The Cognitive Control Task (CCT) was used to assess participants’ level of cognitive control. Participants learned to respond to squared patterns on a computer screen by either pressing left or right key to open a box, and to reveal a food item inside, which was the reward.

A refresher of the rules was given each test day before dose administration, and the accuracy level was tested with the questionnaire (100% correct). Two and a half hours after drug administration, the participants were tested for goal-directed responding (cognitive control).

2.5.1. Profile of mood states

The Profile of Mood States is a self-assessment mood questionnaire with 72 adjectives describing specific mood states. Two composite scales were derived, arousal and positive mood.

2.5.2. Visual analogue scales

Participants indicated on nine Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) how they felt by putting a mark on each 10-cm long horizontal scale. There were five statements and four adjectives presented.

2.5.3. 5-Dimensional altered states of consciousness rating scale

The 5-Dimensional Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale is a 94-item self-report scale that assesses participants’ alterations from normal waking consciousness.

The 5D-ASC consists of five key dimensions and eleven associated subscales: Oceanic boundlessness, Anxious ego dissolution, Visionary restructuralization, Auditory alterations, and Reduction of vigilance.

2.5.4. Ego dissolution inventory

The Ego Dissolution Inventory (EDI) is an eight-item self-report scale that asks participants to indicate to what extent they have experienced a dissolution of their “self” or ego.

2.6.1. The Groninger sleep scale

The Groninger Sleep Scale consists of 15 questions about sleep complaints and 1 question about sleep quantity. The resulting score indicates sleep quality.

2.6.2. LSD plasma concentrations

Blood samples were centrifuged, and pipetted plasma was frozen at -20 °C until analysis for pharmacokinetic analyses. LSD concentrations were determined using ultra high-performance liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry.

2.7. Data and statistical analysis

The data and statistical analysis comply with the recommendations on experimental design and analysis in pharmacology. A General Linear Model (GLM) Univariate Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was performed with Dose and Time as fixed factors, and Participant as a random factor.

For each participant, a different dose of LSD was administered, and a different outcome measure was recorded. Binomial tests were conducted to determine the proportion of observations showing improvement versus impairment.

We used GLM repeated measures (RM) ANOVA with Dose as within-subjects factor to analyze the effects of the 5D-ASC, EDI, and CCT on general health and cognitive function.

Study details

Compounds studied
LSD

Topics studied
Microdosing

Study characteristics
Placebo-Controlled Double-Blind Randomized

Participants
24 Humans

Authors

Authors associated with this publication with profiles on Blossom

Natasha Mason
Natasha Mason is interested in elucidating the neurobiological and cognitive mechanisms of (psychedelic) drugs by utilizing multimodal study designs, with a particular focus on substances that may hold therapeutic value.

Matthias Liechti
Matthias Emanuel Liechti is the research group leader at the Liechti Lab at the University of Basel.

Amanda Feilding
Amanda is the Founder and Director of the Beckley Foundation. She's called the 'hidden hand' behind the renaissance of psychedelic science, and her contribution to global drug policy reform has also been pivotal and widely acknowledged.

Johannes Ramaekers
Johannes Ramaekers is a professor at Maastricht University his work focuses on behavioral toxicology of drugs and combines methods from psychopharmacology, forensic toxicology and neuroscience to determine drug-induced changes in human performance. Some of this research is done with DMT.

Kim Kuypers
Kim Kuypers is a researcher at Maastricht University. Her work is concerned with understanding the neurobiology underlying flexible cognition, empathy, and well-being. One of the main ways she does is with the use of psychedelics.

Institutes

Institutes associated with this publication

Beckley Foundation
The Beckley Foundation is one of the leading voices that has spurred the scientific renaissance of psychedelics research. Led by Amanda Fielding, the NGO funds research and engages with politicians.

Maastricht University
Maastricht University is host to the psychopharmacology department (Psychopharmacology in Maastricht) where various researchers are investigating the effects of psychedelics.

University of Basel
The University of Basel Department of Biomedicine hosts the Liechti Lab research group, headed by Matthias Liechti.

Compound Details

The psychedelics given at which dose and how many times

LSD 5 - 20
μg | 1x

Linked Research Papers

Notable research papers that build on or are influenced by this paper

Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of lysergic acid diethylamide microdoses in healthy participants
This is the third publication of a four-part study. The double-blind, placebo-controlled study (n=23) found that 10 µg of LSD (vs 5 µg) produced psychedelic (psychotropic) effects (even more so at 20 µg). The peak effects were at 2.5h and ended at 5 hours.

Low Doses of LSD Acutely Increase BDNF Blood Plasma Levels in Healthy Volunteers
This is the second publication of a four-part study. It was found that a microdose (20µg) of LSD increased neuroplasticity as measured by brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels at 6 hours (n=24). The results are, however, ambiguous and not present at all values/times.

A low dose of lysergic acid diethylamide decreases pain perception in healthy volunteers
This is the first of a four-part study (n=24) exploring the effects of a microdose of LSD (5-20 µg) on a range of measures, including mood, cognition, empathy, creativity, and physiological parameters. This part of the study found that LSD increased the pain tolerance of participants.

PDF of Mood and cognition after administration of low LSD doses in healthy volunteers: A placebo controlled dose-effect finding study